Keep Your Brain Young


    Read, Be Bilingual, Drink Coffee

    Stephen Krashen says there are three things you can do to stay young mentally and you can do all three at the same time

    Read

    Older people who read more do better on tests of mental ability. In fact they do a lot better. The standard test used to detect dementia is the MMSE, a short test of arithmetic, memory and spatial relations. A research team (Galluccia et al., 2009) found that older people (average age 84) who said they read novels and non-fiction averaged 27.3 on the MMSE, which is in the normal range (27-30). Those who said they only read newspapers averaged 26, which is just below normal. (20-26 = “some impairment,” but those who said
    they did no reading averaged 21, well inside the “impaired” range) Smith (1996) reported that in general older people do not do as well as younger people on reading tests. But older (e.g. age 65 and older) who said they engage in a wide variety of types of reading, or genres (e.g. fiction, current affairs or history, religion, inspiration, science, social science) not only read better than their age-mates who read less widely, but read just as well as younger adults (age 19 to 24) who read just one type of reading material. Smith concludes that “ …extensive reading practice may help to ameliorate possible cognitive declines later in life” (p. 217).
    A popular research design in dementia studies is to test older people who don’t have any signs of problems, and then retest them years later, comparing those who develop problems and those who don’t, called “prospective” studies. In one prospective study, Verghese (et al., 2003) reported that 68 percent of those who developed dementia five years after initial testing said they read books or newspapers frequently (at least several times per week), but 86 percent of those who did not were frequent readers, a significant difference. Geda and colleagues (2009) recently reported similar results.
    One study found that older people (average age 80) were better than younger people (average age 19) on vocabulary and general knowledge, but statistical analysis revealed that age had nothing to do with the difference: The difference was entirely because the older people had read more (Stanovich, West and Harrison, 1995). In the same study, younger people did better on tests of logical thinking and “working memory.” More reading meant somewhat less decline in working memory but not in logical thinking.

    Be Bilingual

    Ellen Bialystok and her colleagues (2007) examined those already diagnosed with dementia. The bilinguals in their sample (those who used two languages on a daily basis since childhood) developed symptoms of dementia about four years later than the monolinguals (age 75.5, compared to 71.4).
    Bialystok and colleagues (2004) also studied why bilingualism helps keep you mentally young. As people get older, they have more difficulty at solving problems that require ignoring irrelevant information and focusing just on important information. In other words, they are more easily distracted. (Now what did I come downstairs for?) Also, younger people are better at keeping information in their memories while solving a problem. Bialystok and associates found that older bilinguals show less of a decline with age than monolinguals in tasks that require keeping information in mind and ignoring distractors. Apparently, the regular use of two languages helps maintain this ability.
    Note: Bialystok’s studies were with those who had been bilingual since youth and who used both languages regularly. We don’t yet know if language acquisition in later life has a positive effect on the brain.

    Drink Coffee

    Prospective studies show that coffee drinkers show less “cognitive decline” as they age: van Gelder (et al., 2007) found that all of their subjects (elderly men) got worse on the MMSE over ten years. But non-coffee drinkers declined more, averaging 2.6 points, while coffee drinkers in general declined 1.4 points. The group that did the best were those who drank three cups a day, declining only 0.6 points, a decline more than four times smaller than the decline experienced by non-drinkers.
    Three more prospective studies found that those who developed Alzheimer’s or dementia were less likely to be regular coffee drinkers preceding the diagnosis. In one five year study, 71 percent who did not develop Alzheimer’s were coffee drinkers, and 57 percent of those who developed Alzheimer’s were (Lindsay et al., 2002), and in another five year study, 67 percent of those considered “cognitively impaired” drank coffee but 76 percent of those who did not were coffee drinkers (Tyas et al., 2001). The difference in this study was not statistically significant, probably because of the small sample size: Only 33 “impaired” subjects were included. Eskelinen (et al., 2009) reported similar results in a 21 year study: The lowest risk for developing dementia and Alzheimer’s was found in those who drank 3-5 cups per day.
    In a retrospective study, one looking back in time, Maia and de Mendonca (2002) reported that Alzheimer’s sufferers consumed an average of between 75 mg of caffeine per day in the 20 years preceding diagnosis. Control subjects, similar subjects without Alzheimer’s, consumed an average of about 200 mg per day.
    Note: The average cup of coffee has between 80 and 175 mg of
    caffeine. A Starbucks tall coffee (12 oz.) has 260 mg.
    Studies with mice (Arendash et al., 2009) suggest that caffeine might be able to reverse the symptoms of Alzheimer’s. Researchers included the equivalent of 500 mg of caffeine (five cups of coffee) in the drinking water of 18-19 month old mice (equivalent to 70 years old in a human) that had been genetically altered to develop memory problems similar to Alzheimer’s as they aged. After two months, the caffeinated mice performed as well as normal mice on tests of memory and thinking. Similar memory-challenged mice who drank plain water did not show any improvement. Also, the caffeinated mice had lower levels of the protein linked to Alzheimer’s (beta amyloid) in both their blood and brains (Cao et al., 2009). Apple juice may also have this effect (Chan and Shea, 2009).
    The research, however, provides no evidence that caffeine improved the memory of normal mice, even if administered from youth through old age. The effect, so far, appears to be specific to dementia. Coffee, in other words, keeps you normal but won’t make you super-normal.
    There is considerable agreement as to the optimal dose of coffee. van Gelder (et al.) reported that the optimal dose to slow cognitive loss was three cups a day (more or less was less effective), and Eskelinen (et al.) report that three to five cups per day was associated with the lowest risk of developing Alzheimer’s. The dose given to mice was about five cups a day (but experimenters did not study the effect of lower doses).
    How about all three together?
    We need to know the effect of combining all three, reading, bilingualism and coffee. Note that it is easy to do them at the same time: Hang out at Starbucks (drink about three regular cups of coffee a day, according to the studies cited), and read a book in another language.
    I would be happy to volunteer as a subject in such a study. Maybe the experimenters will pay for my coffee.

    Stephen Krashen is Professor Emeritus at the Rossier School of Education, USC, Los Angeles, Calif.

    6 COMMENTS

    1. “Bialystok’s studies were with those who had been bilingual since youth and who used both languages regularly. We don’t yet know if language acquisition in later life has a positive effect on the brain.”

      Dear Doctor Krashen, in previous studies done by the scholar mentioned, Dr Ellen Bialystok and colleagues at York University in 2004, http://news.bbc.co.uk/2/hi/health/3794479.stm, the same team concluded the positive effects of bilingualism, but at the same time admitted the “It is also well recognized that education in general can bestow benefits on cognitive function in later life.”
      The acquisition of a language as a second language in later life can imply in itself a great deal of extensive reading performed by the acquirer considering the acquisition of the language being done in some cases for educational purposes, this is, the individual is compelled to performe academic activities like reading novels and academic papers, having to write term papers, attending lectures, and so on.
      I personally consider that the process of acquiring a second language in later life compromises the brain in the same way that when having to use both languages simultaneously since youth. I have seen a great deal of language attrition among many so called bilingual individuals who do not know about language retention and don’t read at all, so reading is the key for both types of bilingual individuals. After all when studying in any specific field of domain, the individual has to read a lot in that field in order to get the knowledge, vocabulary and all the background information, this is the subject matter using the same linguistic device that when acquiring a second language later in life…

    2. Dear Doctor Stephen Krashen:
      I have been attentively reading the report prepared by Doctor Ellen Bialystok, Fergus I.M. Craik, and Morris Freedman about “Bilingualism as a protection against the onset of symptoms of dementia,” and I consider important to mention some details I found in the report:
      (http://www.yorku.ca/coglab/wp-content/uploads/2009/08/Bialystok_Craik_Freedman.pdf)
      (The report was quoted in the BBB
      http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/science-environment-11534481)

      In the report, the researches mentioned the studies done by Valenzuela and Sachdev (2006) and the distinction they make between “neurological brain reserve” and “behavioral brain reserve” which is also referred to as “cognitive reserve.”

      “Valenzuela and Sachdev (2006a) distinguish between, ‘neurological brain reserve’ and ‘behavioral brain reserve.’ Proponents of neurological brain reserve argue that peak brain volume can ameliorate the effects of brain pathology on cognitive performance and signs of dementia. This type of brain reserve is thus presumably biological and possibly genetic in origin. On the other hand, behavioral brain reserve (also referred to as cognitive reserve, the term used in the present report) suggests that sustained complex mental activity protects against dementia in terms of both incidence (Valenzuela & Sachdev, 2006a) and the rate of cognitive decline in elderly individuals (Valenzuela & Sachdev, 2006b).”

      “In their review of behavioral brain reserve, Valenzuela and Sachdev (2006a) found strong evidence for protection against dementia provided by education, high occupational status, high levels of premorbid intelligence, and mentally stimulating leisure activities. Importantly, most of the studies included in the review found significant effects of the protective variable in question after co-varying out age and other brain-reserve measures. One surprising conclusion of the review was that “it is evident that mentally stimulating leisure activity is the most robust brain-reserve measure, since all these studies showed a significant protective effect even after controlling for age, education, occupation and other potential confounds” (Valenzuela & Sachdev, 2006a, p. 447). The authors found an overall decrease in incident dementia of 46% after a median follow-up interval of 7.1 years; these figures are based on a total of over 29,000 individuals from 22 studies.”

      The idea that cognitive reserve really protects people from the early onset of mental decline is well documented in this report aforementioned. My concern is that when Dr. Bialystok and associates studied the bilingual individuals, their sample was only with individuals who were bilinguals since very early age and not since their young adulthood. This is not a fair example of bilingual individuals and I don’t consider the findings to be conclusive. All the individuals were immigrants to have immigrated to Canada and spoke other languages at home and acquired English because of their being in Canada. It is important to notice, as all the doctors in the report say that cultural differences may play an important role when considering medical help, delaying the age of the onset in which the mental condition or decline may be identified.
      I truly consider that much more investigation needs to be done with a variety of bilingual people; this is also with those who have learned a second language in young adulthood and have to use two languages in a daily basis too.

    3. Please…a correction:
      “All the individuals were immigrants “who” have immigrated to Canada and spoke other languages at home and acquired English because of their being in Canada.”

      I found very difficult not to make typo errors in my second language, because of the different sound systems, I think of a word and I sometimes write another. Thank you for your tolerance.

    4. Responding to Jehovanna Arcia Torres’ comments.
      Post of Oct 12: A very good point: The independent contribution of extensive reading, education and second language competence could be teased out with a multivariate study.
      Post of Oct 31: SK: Agreed!! We are all eager to know if second language acquisition, done later in life, has a positive effect on brain health. I think Bialystok’s work is a good step in the right direction.

    5. Thanks for your publication. One other thing is individual American states have their unique laws that affect homeowners, which makes it very, very hard for the our lawmakers to come up with a new set of rules concerning property foreclosures on householders. The problem is that a state features own laws which may have impact in a negative manner in terms of foreclosure plans.

    Comments are closed.